YVETTA FEDOROVA
One tribe is reserved, another wild, when consuming alcohol.
By BENEDICT CAREY
Researchers have had a hard time understanding binge behavior.
Until recently, their definition of binge drinking - five drinks or more in 24 hours - was so loose that it invited debate and ridicule from some scholars.
The dynamics of bingeing may have more to do with personal and cultural expectations than with the number of margaritas consumed.
In their classic 1969 book, “Drunken Comportment, recently out in paperback, the social scientists Craig MacAndrew and Robert B. Edgerton wrote that the disconnect between the conventional wisdom on drunken behavior and the available evidence “is even now so scandalous as to exceed the limits of reasonable toleration.
They detailed the vast differences in the way people from diverse cultures behave after excessive alcohol.
In contrast to nearby tribes, for example, the Yuruna Indians in the Xingu region of Brazil would become exceptionally reserved when rendered sideways by large helpings of alcohol.
The Camba of eastern Bolivia would drink excessively twice a month.
They would toast one another more lavishly with each drink.
In a Japanese island village, Takashima, people knew a drinking occasion had gone wild if villagers began to sing or dance.
Western cultures are more likely to excuse binge drinking as a needed mental break.
In a series of studies in the 1970s and ‘80s, psychologists at the University of Washington observed more than 300 students .
The researchers served alcoholic drinks to some of the students and nonalcoholic ones to others.
The students typically drank five in an hour or two.
The studies found that people who thought they were drinking alcohol behaved exactly as aggressively, or as affectionately, or as merrily as they expected to when drunk.
“No significant difference between those who got alcohol and those who didn’t, Alan Marlatt, the senior author, said.
“Their behavior was totally determined by their expectations of how they would behave.
Somewhere between personal preferences and social custom, moreover, the peer group asserts itself.
In a recent study, public health researchers in New Zealand conducted interviews with teenage girls in one of two cliques at a high school.
Both groups associated drinking with uninhibited behavior - and that is what they exhibited.
But one group considered being uninhibited to include kissing, and the other considered it to include more.
Dr. MacAndrew and Dr. Edgerton acknowledged that Western societies, and certainly the United States, send multiple signals on bingeing.
Other times, cultural expectations and personal preferences reinforce each other.
The hope that a wild session might “reveal new things about myself or “allow me to act completely out of character is echoed in pop culture and drinking lore.
If the research is a guide, those hopes should be self-fulfilling .
Unless, that is, the binge goes beyond any reasonable definition of excess.
Then the amount of alcohol consumed matters - and poison is poison in any culture.
댓글 안에 당신의 성숙함도 담아 주세요.
'오늘의 한마디'는 기사에 대하여 자신의 생각을 말하고 남의 생각을 들으며 서로 다양한 의견을 나누는 공간입니다. 그러나 간혹 불건전한 내용을 올리시는 분들이 계셔서 건전한 인터넷문화 정착을 위해 아래와 같은 운영원칙을 적용합니다.
자체 모니터링을 통해 아래에 해당하는 내용이 포함된 댓글이 발견되면 예고없이 삭제 조치를 하겠습니다.
불건전한 댓글을 올리거나, 이름에 비속어 및 상대방의 불쾌감을 주는 단어를 사용, 유명인 또는 특정 일반인을 사칭하는 경우 이용에 대한 차단 제재를 받을 수 있습니다. 차단될 경우, 일주일간 댓글을 달수 없게 됩니다.
명예훼손, 개인정보 유출, 욕설 등 법률에 위반되는 댓글은 관계 법령에 의거 민형사상 처벌을 받을 수 있으니 이용에 주의를 부탁드립니다.
Close
x